Science
Fact-checked

At InfoBloom, we're committed to delivering accurate, trustworthy information. Our expert-authored content is rigorously fact-checked and sourced from credible authorities. Discover how we uphold the highest standards in providing you with reliable knowledge.

Learn more...

What Is the Representativeness Heuristic?

Michael Anissimov
Michael Anissimov
Michael Anissimov
Michael Anissimov

The representativeness heuristic is a heuristic (rule of thumb) that has been demonstrated to be a natural part of human cognition. Like any other rule of thumb, it has pluses and minuses. The representativeness heuristic argues that people see commonality between items or people of similar appearance, or between an object and a group it appears to be a part of. For instance, a culturally ignorant Westerner might see all brown-skinned people as being part of the same group, despite there being many brown-skinned races without any relation to one another.

The studies that lead to the discovery of the representativeness heuristic were initially conducted by Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman in the early 1970s. Kahneman would later go on to win the 2002 Nobel Prize in Economics. To test for the representativeness heuristic, Kahneman and Tversky gave their subjects the following information:

Woman standing behind a stack of books
Woman standing behind a stack of books

"Tom W. is of high intelligence, although lacking in true creativity. He has a need for order and clarity, and for neat and tidy systems in which every detail finds its appropriate place. His writing is rather dull and mechanical, occasionally enlivened by somewhat corny puns and by flashes of imagination of the sci-fi type. He has a strong drive for competence. He seems to feel little sympathy for other people and does not enjoy interacting with others. Self-centered, he nonetheless has a deep moral sense."

The subjects receiving the information were then divided into three groups, each being given a different decision task:

The first group was asked how similar Tom W. sounded to nine different majors. Most subjects associated him most with an engineering major, and least with a student of social science/social work.

The second group was asked to estimate the probability that Tom W. was a member of any of nine different majors. These probabilities were closely in line with the similarity ratings given by the first group.

The third group was asked to estimate what percentages of first-year grad students were in each of the nine majors, a question completely unrelated to Tom W.

The results indicated that the subjects had a high tendency to assign Tom W. to the engineering group based on representativeness alone, despite the fact that engineering students were relatively rare at the school where the study was conducted, making up substantially less than 1/9 of all students. Being misled based on representations, the subjects ignored the background probabilities of Tom W. being in any given major, notwithstanding any of his personal qualities. Extensive subsequent testing has found that this pathology is universal and applies in a wide variety of problem domains.

The lesson learned from the representativeness heuristic is this: instead of judging something based just on its qualities, consider the background probabilities and try not to make too many assumptions.

Michael Anissimov
Michael Anissimov

Michael is a longtime InfoBloom contributor who specializes in topics relating to paleontology, physics, biology, astronomy, chemistry, and futurism. In addition to being an avid blogger, Michael is particularly passionate about stem cell research, regenerative medicine, and life extension therapies. He has also worked for the Methuselah Foundation, the Singularity Institute for Artificial Intelligence, and the Lifeboat Foundation.

Learn more...
Michael Anissimov
Michael Anissimov

Michael is a longtime InfoBloom contributor who specializes in topics relating to paleontology, physics, biology, astronomy, chemistry, and futurism. In addition to being an avid blogger, Michael is particularly passionate about stem cell research, regenerative medicine, and life extension therapies. He has also worked for the Methuselah Foundation, the Singularity Institute for Artificial Intelligence, and the Lifeboat Foundation.

Learn more...

Discussion Comments

Markerrag

@Logicfest -- what is particularly cynical is how political parties seem to grasp this concept and totally use it to their advantage. If you can cause members who are in sympathy with you to subscribe to a stereotype of an opposing political party that completely demonizes everyone from that party, that translates into votes.

It is unfortunate that representative heuristics are used to gain votes, but that is exactly what is happening. It takes more effort to understand the individual politicians than to just lump them into an inaccurate stereotype. but that's how things have turned out in American politics.

Logicfest

We particularly see this play out in politics. It is common for members of political parties to think of the worst stereotypes for members of the opposition and apply them to the entire group.

Such generalizations are by no means accurate, but it is far easier to make a bunch of assumptions than actually understand the other side.

Post your comments
Login:
Forgot password?
Register:
    • Woman standing behind a stack of books
      Woman standing behind a stack of books